posted by:
|
Brian Drayton
on May 14, 2003
at 8:44PM
|
subject:
|
A really interesting question
|
It could take pages and pages of answers, from many of us, to really come to grips with it. The first thoughts that come to mind: Preamble: The challenge is in the nature of science itself, which is not a single field, but many fields, and each field has some characteristics unusual to itself. They all have in common a general approach: build explanatory theories which provide a causal account of some phenomena, which are in accord with the data, and whose validity is improved by the testing of hypotheses which bear on the (implications of) one or another aspect of the theoretical "story" being woven. If new data appear, or a better way of explaining the data appear, then (hopefully) the explanation becomes more satisfying, gives rise to more implications which upon testing turn out to be true.
But the general approach that I have just sketched is used in the context of stuff, events, phenomena, and these are incredibly complex and various. The learner (and the practitioner) will be required to observe accurately, count/weigh/measure, critique evidence, and decide what variation is worth paying attention to, and what is not. Depending on the phenomena (animal behavior, projectile motion, cloud formations, crystal shapes…) this is a hard thing to do in and of itself. Evidence is often not conclusive, so that all you can say is that the null hypothesis is disproven (for example), not that the alternative hypothesis is proven (or disproven)-- just that one explanation seems more or less likely than it did before the investigation.
And because of the many different kinds of things out there to study, and the many kinds of theories and evidence, there are many kinds of investigations — not everything is experimental, often it's historical (for example, in astronomy, earth science, evolutionary biology…).
So the teacher has to know many different kinds of things. For example, in thinking about weather ( a common middle school topic),: What kinds of phenomena are seen? How are they described (for exmaple, cloud shapes)? What characteristics are used to classify the phenomena? What kinds of data are used to quantify the salient characteristics? How are the data collected? What can go wrong in collecting each kind of data? What are good explanations for some of the phenomena seen, and what theories can be addressed (sometimes even created) by student data collection?
Then there are whatever ideas or assumptions that kids may come in with, or come up with, because most kids have some ideas and experiences (or at least TV acquaintance) with many natural phenomena. How can I tell what sense a kid is making of what she is looking at, or what I am telling her, for that matter? When do I push for qualitative expressions of student ideas, and when quantitative? etc etc etc.
So errors can be of fact (the child did not really see something important, or misunderstood what was happening), or in the way data were collected, or in interpretation, or in reasoning from evidence, or in linking today's learnings with previous learnings…And of course, many of these "errors" should better be seen as transitional understandings, which will change and be replaced in the future, perhaps because the teacher figures he should help the kids test and refine their ideas. (I think it was Bruner who insisted that a child is not an imperfect adult, it is a "thing" in its own right; so too a transitional idea is an idea in its own right, which may be inadequate from various points of view, and need to be addressed withn the agenda of good science learning, but the transitional ideas are often crucial, and part of a teacher's task is to decide when and how to intervene to support, direct, or leave alone the growth of understanding. Not always a decision that can be based on principle…)
|
|